Pheromones (Archive Episode)

49m

Melvyn Bragg and guests discuss how members of the same species send each other invisible chemical signals to influence the way they behave. Pheromones are used by species across the animal kingdom in a variety of ways, such as laying trails to be followed, to raise the alarm, to scatter from predators, to signal dominance and to enhance attractiveness and, in honey bees, even direct development into queen or worker.

With

Tristram Wyatt
Senior Research Fellow at the Department of Zoology at the University of Oxford

Jane Hurst
William Prescott Professor of Animal Science at the University of Liverpool

and

Francis Ratnieks
Professor of Apiculture and Head of the Laboratory of Apiculture and Social Insects at the University of Sussex

Producer: Simon Tillotson

Listen and follow along

Transcript

This BBC podcast is supported by ads outside the UK.

Hey there, it's Ryan Seacrest for Safeway.

Spooky season is quickly approaching, so time to stock up on all your favorite treats.

Now, through October 7th, you can get early savings on your Halloween candy favorites when you shop in-store and online.

Save on items like Hershey's, Reese's, Pumpkins, Snickers, Miniatures, Tootsie Rolls, Raw Sugar, Milk, Chocolate, Caramel, Jack-O'Lanterns, Brock's Candy Corn, Charms Mini Pops, and more.

All friends, October 7th.

Restrictions Restrictions apply, offers may vary.

Visit Safeway.com for more details.

In the market to sell or trade in your luxury exotic, call the company the dealers trust, GimmeTheVin.com.

Looking to sell your Hurricane, 9-11, Cullin Inn, Ferrari, or Bugatti.

Gimme the VIN will pay you the most.

It's as simple as going to GimmeTheVin.com and entering your car's VIN number or license plate number.

You'll get an offer on your car in minutes, well above any dealer quote.

You can go to one of their offices or they'll come to you.

Gimmethevin.com is America's best car buyer.

A happy place comes in many colors.

Whatever your color, bring happiness home with Certopro Painters.

Get started today at Certapro.com.

Each CertaPro Painters business is independently owned and operated.

Contractor license and registration information is available at Certapro.com.

Hello, I'm Simon, producer of In Our Time.

Following Melbyn's announcement that he's stepped down from In Our Time after almost 27 years, we're taking the time to celebrate his outstanding work with some favourite episodes from our archive.

And thanks to everyone who's been in touch.

In due course, we'll return with new programs and a new presenter, but till then, here's Melvin with pheromones from 2019.

Hello, in 1959, scientists discovered pheromones, the chemical signals that make so many animals act without thinking or needing to think.

Ants marching in a line follow pheromones.

Queen bees keep their status in the hive with them.

They're the instant code word for bees to swarm, to attack or flee, and are the great unconscious signal of sexual attraction across so many species.

Insects, fish and mammals use them to advance the cause of their own species.

As for humans, that's debatable, as we'll hear.

With me to discuss pheromones are Tristram Wyatt, Senior Research Fellow at the Department of Zoology at the University of Oxford.

Jane Hurst, William Prescott Professor of Animal Science at the University of Liverpool.

And Francis Rutniaks, Professor of Apiculture and Head of the Laboratory of Apriculture and Social Insects at the University of Sussex.

Tristan Wire, what were the first inklings that there was something that could be called pheromones?

Well, we've probably known about signals between animals right from the beginning of hunting.

And our accounts of the ancient Greeks knowing about the way that dogs attracted each other by smell, not by the bark.

Charles Darwin, in his book, Descent of Man and Sexual Selection, writes, during the season of love, a musky odour is emitted by the glands of the crocodile and pervades their haunts.

And he described smelly ducks, smelly elephants, smelly goats.

But the problem was the quantities were so small they couldn't be identified with the chemistry of the time.

And so how did it develop?

That was an observation, but that didn't start research, I presume.

When did research into it begin?

Well, the serious research started in the 1940s and 50s and it was a German chemist who'd already been awarded the Nobel Prize for his work on the structure of hormones and he spent about 20 years trying to identify the sex pheromone of the silk moth

and he made the breakthrough which was since you couldn't see the molecule, he used the response of the males.

They would flutter their wings when they detected the pheromone and that way he could track the pheromone as he made lots of extractions.

He then isolated the molecule from half a million silk moths, identified it, synthesized it and then the next crucial thing was using that bioassay he went back to the male moths and said have I got this female sex pheromone right?

And they said yes.

It took 20 years and half a million.

These are staggering figures for someone to concentrate on something that seemed obviously so very elusive and difficult.

What instruments did he have?

What did he use?

How did he set about it?

Well basically he was using the behavioral response of the moths as his bioassay, as his indicator.

Incidentally, somebody in the next door lab, Peter Carlson, a biochemist, was living in an apartment next to an early electrophysiologist, Dietrich Schneider.

And in 1953, he suggested to Schneider, well, why don't you try recording with your new electrodes from the moth antenna?

So long before the molecule was actually identified in 59,

Schneider started making electroantenograms, and that basically means using the antenna as the instrument.

And all these sensory hairs get really excited.

They fire off when they detect the pheromone.

And so he was using the antenna as a detector.

But that was too late for the real chemical identification.

That was done just using the fluttering bioassay.

Could you give our listeners a succinct and unforgettable definition of a pheromone?

It's invisible chemical signals between members of the same species.

Well, that does it.

Thank you very much.

Francis, Francis, Brittany, why are the pheromones so important to insect colonists?

Well, the animals, the insects that live in colonies like honeybees and ants, probably of all the animals, they use pheromones the most.

They use them not just in the case of, say, the silk moth, so that the female can attract a male.

Indeed, the honey bee does have such a pheromone so that the queen bee can attract the drone bees on a mating flight.

So that they do, but they do a whole lot of other stuff with their pheromones.

For example, honeybees use pheromones to help in the regulation of foraging, to the regulation of reproduction, to organise their defense when their colony is attacked, to help bees who got lost find their way back to the hive entrance.

There's just so many things that they use pheromones for.

Honeybees have about 20 different communication signals to coordinate the thousands of workers in the colony.

And more than half of those communication signals are pheromones.

And actually, new ones are kind of being discovered.

I wouldn't say all the time, but on a fairly regular basis, we're discovering new communication signals in honeybees.

And the same kind of applies to ants as well.

They use use a wide range of pheromones.

I think the social insects, they are the sort of prime beneficiaries, if you like, of pheromones.

So, this is just a particular smell that comes out of a chemical action?

The pheromones?

Well, the pheromones say that the honeybees produce, yes, they're all chemicals.

All the pheromones we talk about.

So, this is just released into the air and they're picked up by those that are intended to be picked up by mates or the rest of the colony or whatever it is.

Some of the pheromones indeed are released into the air, but certainly not all of them.

The queen pheromone, for example, in the context of mating, yes, the male bees do detect it in the air, but the primary way it's detected is by touching or licking the queen and also passing it around the colony.

But some of the pheromones are passed in the air.

So, the alarm pheromone of the honeybee, if you were to be so foolish as to go to a bee colony and disturb the hive, and one of the bees at the entrance, one of the guard bees, if you molested it, it might well sting you.

And that would be one sting, and many people wouldn't like that.

But the stinger itself will release a pheromone,

and that pheromone smells of bananas and it wafts through the air very quickly because it's very volatile.

And then other bees will come out of the hive and continue the attack.

So, what started as one sting might be many.

So, the advice is to pull out the sting as fast as possible?

Well, it's probably too late by then.

You should pull it out, but I think you'll be slower than the bees response.

So

that's a sort of defence mechanism that they're using it for.

Yes, in this case, the pheromone is used in the organisation of the defence of the colony.

And if you think about it, a social insect colony like a honeybee hive does have something to defend, whereas if it was a solitary insect, it would maybe fight for its own life or flee for its own life, but it wouldn't really have anything that it would need to do to call up support.

Whereas the insect colony, say a honeybee colony, has got a treasure of food inside of it.

You know, tens of kilograms of young bees, honey, many predators would love to get in and plunder that.

And equally, they've got thousands of bees to mount a defence.

So you can

be stung by a thousand honey bees, especially the so-called killer bees from Africa.

And so they can mount a pretty decent defence.

Just to flip back a few centuries, the the interest in bees has been constant in classical literature, right through it in Shakespeare.

Were people noticing these things in their own ways of expressing them rather than the ways you could express them as chemicals and so on?

People, the ancients, you know, Pliny the Elder, he knew a lot about honeybees, Aristotle.

Not everything Aristotle said was correct.

But because honey bees produce honey and beeswax, which is so important to people, there was a huge amount of interest in bees.

But if we come only to roughly 1620 or 30, a British person, Charles Butler, wrote a book called The Feminine Monarchy.

He was actually, the first 30 years of his life was the time of Queen Elizabeth, so maybe he had female monarchs on his mind.

But he made some observations.

If a bee stings you, what he referred as to the rank odour.

In fact, I don't think it's a rank odour, it's of bananas.

The alarm pheromone of the honeybee smells of bananas.

I wouldn't call that a rank odour.

And then he said it would cause an attack, which would be like hail.

And so I think he had a particularly nasty beehive.

So that was, well, 1620s.

So that's nearly 400 years ago.

People were talking about things which we now know to be pheromones.

And you've told us how quickly they can react together.

Janehurst, what is it about mammals that make them less likely to use pheromones?

Well, mammals actually

find scent cues really important, and scent cues are used in most aspects of their biology and behaviour.

But But the sort of pheromone cues that Francis has just been talking about, the sort of classical pheromone, is not just

a signal between two animals of the same species, it's a signal that will normally produce a sort of stereotyped response, a very reliable response.

And those sorts of responses that Francis has just been talking about, you can reiterate those responses just by producing this pheromone cue.

Now, mammals are a little bit more complicated than that.

They've evolved large brains

and they are able to process and store an awful lot of information and learn from their previous experiences.

And that's so that they can adjust their behaviour according to the information they've learnt before and any information that they've gained about other individuals in their environment and the relationships they've built up.

So, when we start to think about pheromonal responses, which are just sort of fairly simple responses, reliable responses to a particular cue, it doesn't really fit in with that idea of adjustable behaviour, where animals can respond very differently according to what they've learned about other animals.

And indeed, there's been quite a lot of

discussion about whether really mammals use pheromones much at all

to regulate their behaviour.

And some scientists have said they don't believe mammals use pheromones that actually have these very reliable effects on behaviour.

However, we have now identified some sort of classic pheromones that are used by mammals and we're starting to understand how those pheromones can be used in the much more flexible behavioural system of mammals.

Including us?

Possibly.

I think perhaps we'll get to that later.

It's probably easier to start by thinking about some rather simpler situations in mammals and in particular situations perhaps where mammals haven't had an opportunity to learn, such as newborn animals.

In particular, they have to be able to find milk if they're going to survive.

And one of the first really clear examples of a mammalian pheromone is the rabbit mammary pheromone, which was identified by Benoit Charles and Gérard Couraud and their group in France.

And it's a small aldehyde which

female rabbits produce in their milk.

And they were able to show that by synthesizing this one little aldehyde and they presented it to newborn rabbit kits and they actually did this by putting the aldehyde on the end of a glass rod and held it in front of the nose of the pups.

What happens then is you see this very stereotype response which is the pups start rapidly searching around with their noses and start grasping and try to grasp hold of what essentially should be a nipple.

And that it's a response that is just stimulated by this very, very simple chemical and is essential for these pups to be able to feed very efficiently of their mother.

Particularly important in rabbits because rabbit mothers actually only visit the nest for a few minutes each day and the pups have to grab hold of the nipple and get as much milk as they can from the female before she disappears again.

So you need a very very efficient response and of course they haven't had very much time to learn how to do that.

So that's the beginning.

Would you say that's a simple example and there are more complex ones that you could produce?

It's a simple example but actually gets a little bit more complicated because the other interesting thing they discovered about that pheromone is that the actual pheromone actually induces the pups to learn and they learn other cues that are associated with the pheromone.

So, if they, as they, as when they're exposed to the pheromone, if they're also exposed to another odour, they learn to respond to that odour in the same way.

So, initially, they wouldn't respond, say, to just the general odour of their mother,

but once they've associated the odour of their mother with this milk pheromone, then now when they encounter that odour, they'll start to show the same kind of searching behaviour.

And in fact, if you watch a female rabbit going into her nest, you see the whole nest erupting with these baby rabbits starting to search really rapidly and try and get hold of the nipples as quickly as they can.

Thank you.

Tristram White, what role have these pheromones played in evolution or evolution in pheromones?

Could you give us a dissertation on that?

So one of the surprising things is that almost any kind of molecule can become a pheromone.

So we've heard about some small molecules, these volatile alarm pheromones and that volatile rabbit mammary pheromone.

Underwater the pheromone molecules can be huge, they can be proteins, so long as they're soluble.

So in different habitats, in different places different kinds of molecules evolve to be pheromones.

And in some cases we can have a feeling, an understanding of how molecules that are just around, as it were, just leaking out, can evolve into pheromones.

So goldfish pheromones were discovered by accident.

Scientists in Canada were studying hormones in goldfish and the water was flowing from one tank into the next.

When the upstream tank had female goldfish that were getting hormone treatments, the male goldfish downstream got very excited and started producing lots of sperm.

And it turned out that the goldfish pheromones were based, were like the hormones that were coursing in the blood of the female goldfish.

So the female sex pheromones were based on the hormones.

And what Peter Sorensen and Norm Stacey hypothesize is that way back in evolutionary time, males that responded quickly were sensitive to these molecules, which were basically just cues in the environment, would reach the female first.

There'd be selection over the generations for greater and greater sensitivity, but also also specificity, so they didn't have false alarms.

So that starts as eavesdropping, but it's to the female's advantage to attract a male to be there to deliver sperm when she spawns.

So it turns into a signal when females evolve to produce these molecules as a signal, as a pheromone.

So that's one of the ways that pheromones can evolve.

Is there an evolutionary history there?

It's what you said at the end of some development or halfway through some deportative development.

Is it still still developing?

Well, I'm using the word develop, mutating, whatever you'll find a better word.

Pheromones can certainly evolve.

They're used in pest control, and we'll hear about that later.

And so for the...

Well, I wouldn't want to settle this, so you can tell us now.

Okay.

So one of the strange things is that the first New York State entomologist in the 1880s anticipated that pheromones would be powerful.

So he watched male moths flying into his window

and people were gathering on the sidewalk and he hypothesized in the 1880s that if only we could synthesize these molecules we could perhaps use their power to control insect pests which were a big thing in North America.

Fast forward to the first pheromones being identified in Germany rapidly people started identifying the molecules and trying them out in the field.

And if you release enough of these synthetic pheromones, the males can't find the females,

they don't fertilize the females, without fertile eggs, no caterpillars, crops protected.

And most of the apples we buy today have had some pheromone protection.

And so that's one example of evolution.

And that's evolution, but that's manipulated evolution.

Now, that's manipulated evolution, and what we might expect if pheromones work like pesticides, that we get resistance.

And so far we haven't had any resistance.

So is this a good thing?

A very good thing.

And particularly because the pheromones are used in relatively small quantities because the moths don't need very much to be confused.

And because they don't affect predators, we leave intact all the spiders and the predatory beetles.

So you can have organic farming that works really well.

Thank you.

Francis,

what's the queen substance and why is that important?

Well, the queen substance is

dominating.

The queen's substance is perhaps the only pheromone which has a common name that a man in the street or woman in the street might have heard of.

And

so before

it was, if you like, chemically discovered, people knew that there was something going on.

So a British scientist, another butler actually, but this time Colin Butler, he hypothesized that the queens were making a certain material which he called queen substance.

And without knowing what it was, he set out to learn about it.

And it's known to beekeepers that if you take the queen out of a colony, very quickly within a few hours and certainly within half a day or a day, the worker bees know that the queen is absent.

And they set about maybe trying to find the queen.

But what they do, and what Butler used to study the queen substance, was that they'll start making a new queen.

And this can be observed because what they do is they take a larva, a female larvae in a worker cell, these are these famous hexagonal cells that everybody knows about, and then they would try to, if you like, promote a few larvae into queens.

And to do that, they have to feed the larvae with royal jelly, and they also modify the cell, making it looking like an acorn cup.

Just a second, they're acting like you, Lord, aren't they?

They're acting like scientists.

They're doing an experiment

on this bee

to change the bee.

You say.

They're doing Frankenstein, they're trying to.

I mean, for somebody like me who knows nothing about it, it's amazing.

So

they haven't got a queen bee, they want another, they pick up some, the you're going to be the next queen bee, and then they take things from other bees and inject, project, penetrate.

How do they get them in this other bee?

So it turns into a queen beef.

Well, fairly simply, no, when they feed, they feed the larvae and then force feed them.

No, they don't force feed them.

Honey bee larvae actually float in their food, so it's a bit like being in a bath of food, and the honey bee larva is floating in its bath of food secreted by the worker bees.

So they're kind of lying in bee milk and if you want to become a queen you have to be lying in the special bee milk we call royal jelly.

Anyway, this construction of these queen cells or queen cups was the bioassay that Colin Butler used.

So he could basically study the queen substance.

He didn't know what it was.

And one of the experiments he did, for example, was to put a a queen in a cage so the worker bees couldn't touch her, but the air could pass over.

And if he did that, the worker bees felt as though they didn't have a queen.

So he knew, therefore, that the queen's substance was not volatile.

Tristram?

Well, I wanted to pick up on the royal jelly, which was mentioned by the two scientists who coined the word pheromone in 1959.

So one of them was a termite biologist, the other was a biochemist Peter Carlson.

But they consulted widely including writing to Colin Butler about the queen substance.

But they anticipated that there might be some different kinds of pheromones that would actually act by mouth.

So most of the pheromones we've been talking about so far are detected by the nose, whether in the mouse or the antennae of a bee or a moth.

There are other pheromones which are ingested and act directly on tissues in the developing bee larva, in this case, and those are called allohormone pheromones.

There's a bit of debate about whether we should include them, but in the sense that they're released by one animal, the nurse bee, and given to another and have an effect, they actually count as a pheromone.

So it's a particular kind of pheromone that acts not on the normal sense organs, but actually acts directly on the tissues.

There's a moment every parent remembers, the day their child takes off on two wheels.

With Guardian bikes, that moment comes as early as two years old and with less stress and frustration.

These bikes are built just for kids.

Lightweight frames, low center of gravity, easy to use brakes.

Everything about Guardian is designed to help kids ride confidently, often in just one day, no training wheels needed.

And because Guardian bikes are designed and assembled right here in the USA, you know they're built to last with care in in every detail, their patented sure-stop braking system stops both wheels with a single lever, helping your child stop safely without tripping forward or losing control.

Right now, save hundreds when comparing Guardian to its competitors at GuardianBikes.com and get a free lock and pump when you join their newsletter of $50 value.

Visit GuardianBikes.com today to save and help your child learn an essential life skill, safely.

Guardian Bikes, built for your kid and for the memories you'll never forget.

GiveMeTheVin.com is the best place to sell your classic or collectible, whether it's one car or an entire collection.

GiveMeTheVin want your muscle car, hot rod, or your old square body pickup, and they love buying resto mods.

Go to givemeTheVin.com today to sell your classic or collectible and avoid sellers' commissions, tire kickers, and low ballers on the internet.

It's as simple as going to givemethevin.com and entering your car's VIN number or license plate number.

GivemeTheVin.com is America's best car buyer.

Hey there, it's Ryan Seacrest for Safeway.

Spooky season is quickly approaching, so time to stock up on all your favorite treats.

Now, through October 7th, you can get early savings on your Halloween candy favorites when you shop in-store and online.

Save on items like Hershey's, Reese's, pumpkins, Snickers, miniatures, Tootsie Rolls, raw sugar, milk, chocolate, caramel, jack-o'-lanterns, Brock's candy corn, charms, mini pops, and more.

All friends, October 7th.

Restrictions apply.

Offers may vary.

Visit safeway.com for more details.

Jane Hurst, can we switch to mice?

Okay.

How do they deal with how do they use pheromones?

Pheromones.

Well it's kind of quite interesting really because you might think that you know having had the example of the rabbits it's an obvious example the pups need to find mother's milk and they haven't had any experience they use a pheromone but actually when people looked at mice it turns out not to be the case.

In the case of mice actually the pups imprint on the maternal odour, the individual odour of their own mother, and they find the nipples by following the odour of their own mother.

So when they're actually born, the mother cleans them off, and that stimulation of the pups stimulates the pups to learn the mother's odour.

And they'll learn the odour of the mother's saliva, and they also

learn the odour of the amniotic sac that they've been sitting in.

And then the mother distributes that odour on her nipples, and that's what the pups will respond to.

But if you give them the odour of another mother, they don't respond to that.

So, here we have a social animal, and I think the big difference here is that in the case of rabbits, individual females have different nests, the pups only need to recognise their own mother, they can use a simple pheromone.

In the case of the mice, mice are social, and there are other mice using the nest.

So, it's very likely that this imprinting on their own mother allows them to get to their own mother's milk.

And, of course, we see that sort of response in many other mammals, where it's very much an individual-specific response.

There have been

most of the pheromones that have been actually identified in mice, these things which don't depend on which individual produces it, but it's a generalized response, they're mostly involved in what we call reproductive priming pheromones.

So, these are odors that will change the hormonal status of an animal, usually to make it more or less likely to mate.

And we do know of quite a few of those pheromones.

But actually, recently, we have discovered

another pheromone in mice that really does have an effect on their behaviour.

And it's a pheromone that we've called Darsin.

You're plundering Jane Austen there.

Yes.

So this is a pheromone.

I'll explain this to the listeners.

I don't think I'm being too clever by half.

I read it in your thing, and that's where I got it.

Right.

So, well, the thing with mice is that they might use sort of fairly simple pheromones in the sort of context of reproduction, but when it comes down to mate choice, that's very much more an individual-specific response.

And females are really choosy in who they choose to mate with.

And what males will do is that they will scent mark their territories with a lot of scent marks that they stuff full with little proteins which actually advertise their own individual identity.

So each male produces its own little pattern of proteins.

These proteins are called mups or major urinary proteins.

And they're trying to advertise to females who they are.

And they will compete with other males.

If other males come and try and also deposit scent marks, they will countermark that male scent marks and try and drive them out of their territory.

So they're really kind of communicating to females, not just I'm male, but I'm this male and this is the male that you should mate with.

Well females are quite interested in these ODQs, but they don't really find them very attractive.

So they will investigate them, but they don't actually find them more attractive than, say, an individual signature from a female.

That is until they actually contact the male's scent and when they contact the male scent they detect another little protein pheromone.

It's another mup but it has a slightly different sequence from the others and unlike the individual specificity of the signatures that the males are producing, this is a protein which all males produce and this protein females find really attractive.

And we've sequenced the protein, if we just give the female that protein, they're really, really attracted to it.

But the really interesting thing with this protein is that it doesn't just attract them on its own, it's another one of these proteins that is super potent in stimulating learning in the females.

And the moment they have a sniff of that protein, they learn the individual ode cue of the male that's associated with it, and now they find that individual male's odour really attractive.

And that's called the Darcin protein.

That's called the Darsin protein.

And you sat around a table and decided that

this protein would be named after Darcy.

We've got to come to a conclusion on this.

So, actually, well, not so much a table.

I have to say,

we sat with a bottle of wine.

And my husband and collaborator on this, he's a protein biochemist also at Liverpool.

And he said that you could name a protein anything you liked, but usually it has the suffix in on the end in.

And there'd been a couple of other examples.

There's a protein pheromone that newts use, which they emit when they're

courting females to attract them, and that's been called Sudefrin, which is actually after the Japanese.

Could you tell us about Darcy and then we could move on?

I probably

a bit of a red hot red pheromone in there.

So, Darcy Me named after Jane Austen's romantic hero, Mr.

Darcy, in Pride and Prejudice.

And it was really inspired by the first line of the book, which I'm sure you know.

It's a truth universally universally acknowledged that a single man in possession of a good fortune must be in want of a wife.

Well, of course, a good fortune isn't particularly important to a female mouse, but it does appear that a single male mouse in possession of Darsin must be in want of a mate.

Great.

Tristram.

Can we talk about the context in which they usually talk about the chemistry we've talked about in the literature?

What about the context?

It matters a lot.

One of the interesting things about insect pheromones is that it was often assumed that this was a completely stereotypical response and males would always respond in the same way, or for that matter, females in those situations.

But it's actually much more subtle.

And in the same way as in

mammals, context is everything

in certain cases.

So for example, turnip moth males get very excited by the female sex pheromone, but once they've mated,

they turn off for 24 hours.

Now, if you record from the antenna, that electroantenogram again, the antenna are receiving the signal, but in the brain, the response is switched off.

It's context dependent.

And the reason for that is when he mates with the female, he's going to give a whole load of proteins along with the sperm, and it takes him 24 hours to produce them.

And so he shouldn't respond until he's got them ready.

Also, in moths, the male moth, when he's flying to the female at night, is in danger of being eaten by bats.

So they try and leave it as long as possible, but then they do have to fly to find the female if they're going to mate.

But their willingness to fly depends on whether or not they've heard bats calling.

And so they'll fly faster if they have, and they'll sometimes even shut it down.

So lots of things are, even in insects, context dependent.

Francis, can we talk about ants?

How do they deploy more when they're out and about and moving, as they seem to be doing incessantly?

Well, one of the commonest things you can see is ants following a trail.

And what ants do, say an ant finds some food, on the way back to the nest it may lay a pheromone trail by either dragging its sting or dotting its abdomen to lay a chemical trail.

And back in the ant colony, it will then sort of stimulate other ants to go foraging, and they will be able to follow this pheromone trail to the food.

So that's actually something that honeybees don't do.

And ants have a huge repertoire of trail pheromones.

And in our own work, for example, we discovered in one pest ant which lives in Britain that they really have three trail pheromones.

One is very short-lived, only lasts for minutes, and we another one which is much longer-lived.

And to detect that one, the ants actually have to not smell, if you like, but taste.

They have to touch the substrate with their antennae because it's less volatile, presumably.

And the third one, maybe the most interesting of all, one of my former students now at York University, Elva Robinson, she discovered a no-entry pheromone.

So, this is not saying come this way, but don't come this way, because ant trails frequently have branches in them.

So, it's not just a matter of going to the right branch, it can also be important to be told don't enter this particular branch.

So, there's a great deal of sophistication going on in ant colonies and their pheromone trails.

And of course, as everybody knows, you have a picnic or you spill some food, and the next thing there's ants all over the place.

Jane Hurst,

how have animals adapted to detect pheromones using something more than a sense of smell?

Ah, okay.

So, as humans, of course, we do think of the sense of smell as being sense that you can detect, the airborne scents that you detect when you breathe them into your nose through what we call the main olfactory epithelium, which is at the back of the nose.

But actually, most other mammals, reptiles, and amphibia have evolved another olfactory system.

It's called the vomeronasal organ.

And this is a separate system.

Unfortunately, humans don't have it.

So we really don't know quite what it's like to sniff something through the vomeronasal organ.

Humans don't have it, great apes don't, and old world monkeys don't have this, at least not a functioning system as far as we can tell.

But this is a rather different system.

So the way that our main olfactory system works is that the receptors are quite broadly tuned.

It allows us to detect a whole range of different sorts of odours, not just scents, but all the odours in your environment.

Whereas the vomer nasal organ, the receptors there are tuned very finely to detect only a single chemical.

So each receptor detects one particular chemical and those will be chemicals which have particular biological significance to that species.

So those could be pheromones which are important for communication within species, but they could also be molecules from other species.

So, for example, predators evolve receptors for key molecules from their prey, and vice versa.

So, this is a very specialized system, and it was at one time thought that all pheromones would be detected through this vomoronasal system.

But we do know now that some volatile airborne pheromones can be detected through the main olfactory system.

Thank you, Tristram.

How do some animals use this to deceive?

So

if you rely on chemicals, then you can have illegal signallers.

And there's a fabulous story about bolus spiders in North America.

The female web spider, in this case,

puts her web into a sticky ball at the end of a long thread.

So it looks like a bolus, just like the gauchos in South America.

She synthesizes, as it were illegally, she counterfeits the pheromone, the female sex pheromone of moths.

Male moths fly up wind at night and she detects when they come close by the vibrations as they fly, and she then throws her ball, the bolas, catches the male moth and draws it in.

It gets weirder.

At different times of night, she releases different counterfeit blends.

So that way she can pretend to be

the the female moth and lure them in like a siren.

It's extraordinary.

And you have pictures.

I mean have people can you photograph this?

Patiently yes.

So there's a biologist called Ken Haynes in the University of Kentucky who's got some wonderful photographs of this.

There are other examples where

groups,

families of beetle larvae, group together, pretend to be a bee, and they produce the smell of a female solitary bee.

The male tries to mate with this

apparently a female bee, they jump on and then they jump off when he actually does find a real female.

You're trying to come in.

Well you can go to YouTube and see a video of the bolas spider and I think none other than David Attenborough introducing it.

So and indeed I show that to my students in one of my animal behavior lectures.

But we also have the reverse case where in going back to honey bees there's a honeybee species living in Asia very similar to our Western honeybee, and it has a really important predator, the giant hornet.

And the giant hornet has a pheromone.

So if one giant hornet finds a honeybee nest to plunder, it releases a pheromone, which attracts more giant hornets, its nestmates, to the attack of the victim colony.

Now, what the honeybees do is they also detect this pheromone.

and they then mount a very specialized anti-hornet defence

and they try and capture the hornet, surround it with a ball of bees, and cook it to death.

And we call that a chiramone.

I'd like to know more about that, please.

I'm a captain,

cook it to death, right?

That's

it.

You've seen that again and again.

It's news to me, so it'd be lovely to know more about that.

If you've been watching enough television, you've probably been too much on the radio, but again, you can see good videos on this.

Well, the giant hornet is very, very an armoured animal, and the honey bee stings just can't penetrate it.

But the honey bees form a dense ball of maybe 500 worker bees around the hornet, and they raise their temperature and they literally cook the hornet to death because the honey bees can sustain and stand a higher temperature than the hornet.

But the point is, the honeybees

get warning of the hornet by eavesdropping on the hornet's own communication to its nestmates.

And there are a few other cases where that happens, where a pheromone of a predator is detected by the prey, who then can mount a special defence.

Janehurst, which ways do males and females respond to the same phenomenon in

same pheromones in mammals?

So, typically, of course, males and females are going to respond differently to any kind of sex pheromone.

They have very different meanings and they need to show different types of responses.

So, I guess traditionally, we've sort of thought, oh, a pheromone has a specific response, it's evolved to have that response, and then all other animals that are going to respond to it will respond in that way.

But actually that's not the case when you start looking at different types of individuals.

So if we look for example at Darsin, Darsin has evolved as a signal to females to attract females to the male.

And of course males are not passive in this.

When males detect Darsin from other males, it makes them highly competitive.

So Darsin will stimulate a competitive countermarking response and it actually stimulates aggression against the sender.

Now of course it wouldn't be appropriate to really think of that as an aggression pheromone because it's not likely that a male will have evolved a pheromone that causes other males to attack it.

However it has evolved as a pheromone to attract females and so the appropriate response from males, if you like, they're eavesdropping in the same way that we've just been talking about species eavesdropping on the sense of other species.

In this case males are eavesdropping on the signals that other males are producing to attract females and responding to that in a very competitive way.

And it's a race to the

race to set,

competition sets in.

It is very much a big stimulus for competition.

But actually, even within the same sex, we can see different responses to the same pheromone according to the state of the animal.

So, for example, while Darsin attracts females that don't have offspring, if they're lactating and a a male comes in expressing Darsin, females will attack it if they're not familiar with it.

Can we come back to humans, Tristram?

We said that we had no...

Well, let's say humans have pheromones, but we're too complicated, busy, or occupied to make any use of them.

Is that all we have to say about it?

No, it's an ever-interesting story.

Our sense of smell is very good.

We're very good smellers.

The problem is there's a lot of bad science concerning human pheromones.

I'm persuaded that we probably do have them, but if you search for pheromones on the internet, you'll get lots of sites offering you pheromones for sale.

They tend to be some steroid molecules, they're basically made up.

The same group though that has been working on rabbit pheromones in France is also working on a possible human pheromone.

So the sex pheromones, we probably might have them, but none properly identified.

But this group in France is looking at something different and this is a potential mammary pheromone and it looks as though baby humans, human babies, respond to secretions from the nipple, around the nipple of lactating mothers, these Montgomery or areola glands, the little bumps around the nipple.

And it looks as though a secretion from any mother will cause any baby to respond.

And that, I think, may be the first human pheromone to be identified.

The things on the internet, they could have a placebo effect, but they're not real.

Can you just say a little bit more about the things on the internet?

These are advertisements to

empower your sex life by using pheromones.

They promise to make you irresistible.

I see.

And I guess if you...

Darcy again.

Indeed, if only we had a Darcin.

Although, well, humans can do things in other ways.

Jane.

Yes, there is quite a big difference, actually, in the way that mammals like mice are using these pheromones.

Because I guess if we take the idea of a classic pheromone, this is a signal that will produce a very reliable stereotype response.

And that's generally not what mammals want to do.

And the way that mice are using this pheromone is to actually have choice over whether they're exposed to the pheromone or not.

I think that's a very important thing about the vomeronasal system, because in order to detect something in the vomer nasal system, animals have to actively deliver the scent to the system.

Whereas if you detect something in your main olfactory system, you're exposed to it simply if you just breathe air into your nose.

And I think that's potentially a reason why it would actually be quite hard to think of a sexual attraction pheromone working in humans, because actually, most people don't want to be just attracted to every

person of the opposite sex that they meet.

They need to have some kind of a choice.

They're using a lot more information to make those choices.

Do you think that you're at at the beginning of research

in this area, Francis?

In the case of pheromones, the honeybee

situation, maybe 50 years ago they thought they understood it.

I'd say today, you know, we are learning so much and there's so much new stuff coming in.

There are so many pheromones, how do they work together?

We still, in many ways, are still at the beginning.

Well, thank you very much for that.

That was terrific.

Thank you.

Thank you, Francis.

Ratniaks, Tristram Wyatt, and Jane Hurst.

And next week, it's Antari ibn Shatdad and the poetry of the Arabian Peninsula before Islam.

Thank you for listening.

And the In Our Time podcast gets some extra time now with a few minutes of bonus material from Melvin and his guests.

This programme is called In Our Time.

And Colin Butler, the discoverer, one of the main people in the discovery of honeybee queen substance, well, he only died three years ago.

And I think that shows you that it's all pretty recent.

And another element of the recency is that in the time of Butinant and the discovery of the silk moth, he used half a ton of moths.

Now, chemistry has evolved to such a degree that one moth would be more than sufficient.

Maybe just

a glass full of air around the female moth might be enough.

So the chemistry has really helped the study of pheromones a great deal, the much more sophisticated analytical chemistry we now have.

Because Burton Dent didn't have the gas chromatography and the mass spectrometry that modern chemists have, and that has revolutionised the whole field.

It's funny, chemistry held things back and it's another example of how

science moves by new techniques being developed and suddenly you can do new things.

We actually got very close with observations back in the 1870s.

There was a French entomologist, naturalist really, Jean-Marie Farbre,

and he was another of these people describing male moths flying in through the window.

And he went through this logical series of experiments.

So he tried covering up the female under a gauze.

The males still found her.

He took the cotton wool that she'd been resting on.

The males flew to the cotton wool.

And he's moving towards the idea that it's probably smell.

So he tries covering her under a glass.

The males are not interested so he concludes that it must be smell and then he makes the fatal error.

He assumes that any smell that is important for sex is something that we can smell.

So he sniffs the female moths and he can't smell anything so he rules it out and what he forgets of course is the male moths are exquisitely sensitive to those molecules.

Now if he'd only made the right conclusion it would have brought things forward.

But we still would have had to wait for half a million salt moths.

And I think, actually, that's also been the case in terms of mammalian pheromones, because we've always assumed that they are these volatile molecules that we can smell.

And it's turned out that an awful lot of these pheromones are proteins.

And of course, humans can't smell proteins at all.

And you can only detect these when they're actually delivered to this special vomeronasal system.

And I think, actually, while we also need

the chemical techniques, the other really important thing in these areas is having a really good bioassay and having a bioassay that actually measures realistic behaviour.

And I think that's probably the best way that we're going forward now in understanding the much more sophisticated effects that these pheromones are having.

And that's actually, of course, the thing that's holding back the study of human pheromones.

The nice thing about the work on babies and the potential mammary pheromone is they have a nice bioassay, which is the baby turning its head, puckering its lips, even sticking out its tongue in response to the potential molecules.

When we come to sex pheromones in humans, it's really hard.

There are so many cultural things and trying to get those kind of experiments through an ethics committee don't even start.

So it's going to be very hard to get good bioassays and that's another thing which has held back all the work on human sex pheromones, if indeed they exist.

Do you have anything to offer on that?

I I was going to say to Jane, maybe you could tell us what the ideal experiment would be for a human sex attraction.

Well, of course.

I was only joking, really.

But it might be a fun thing to think about.

And assuming we can get permission, so it's not a.

Well, I think one of the things that I really noticed the difference in the human literature, I think there are two things.

First, that many people working with human pheromones have a very different definition of pheromones to those of us who work with animals.

And I think this idea of a pheromone that stimulates a very specific response is a really good one because actually we can see this time and time again in animals.

Whereas a lot of people working with human pheromones would accept any kind of response, even if it's a learned response, if it's just something that arises through cultural

responses.

And I think the thing we particularly have to look for in humans are those situations where it's a really reliable response, because when you see responses to the purported pheromones that have been tested, you know, some people respond to it, some people don't.

Well, actually, if we test any of these pheromones on my mice, I can guarantee you they will respond.

And I think that's the whole point about pheromones.

It's a reliable response.

It's a signals that evolved to really produce this type of response to the advantage of both signal and receiver.

And so, if we're going to try and look for human pheromones, I think we have to be very clear about what kind of response we would expect to see, so we know what we're trying to measure, and we should expect, if it's a pheromonal response, to see that response really reliable, shown by people.

We're also rather fixated, a bit like the advertisements for deodorants for young men, on long-distance attraction.

And if humans did have sex pheromones, they might occur at a much later stage in courtship, perhaps in really quite intimate situations.

And so it's going to be very interesting indeed.

So they wouldn't work in a crowded pub at 11 o'clock at night on a Friday then?

No, I think we're we're seven.

So your herbs dashed, Darling.

Not really, Melvin Herb, but seven.

I suppose if everybody had the pheromone, it would all cancel out, wouldn't it?

You know, we'd all be back to square one.

Well, the alcohol would interfere with the pheromone, perhaps.

Or would it?

Probably not.

Well, thank you all very much.

That was really enjoyable.

Thank you.

How would you respond to an offer of tea or coffee?

Tea, please.

Coffee, please.

Black tea week, please.

In our time with Melvin Bragg is produced by Simon Tillotson, and it's a a BBC Studios production.

I'm Hannah Fride and I'm Dara O'Brien and we are back for another series of Curious Cases where we investigate the scientific mysteries sent in by you.

Are crows capable of complex emotions?

What happens to our brains when we fall in love?

And I was wondering why do we lie?

I think that one might be aimed at you, Dara.

How would you know?

That's what a liar would say.

We tackle the mysteries of the universe through audacious experiments and expert insight.

Curious Cases on Radio 4 and available now on BBC Sounds.

It's time your hard-earned money works harder for you.

With the Wealthfront Cash Account, your uninvested cash earns a 3.75% APY, which is higher than the average savings rate.

No account fees, no minimums, and free instant withdrawals to eligible accounts anytime.

Join over a million people who trust Wealthfront to build wealth at wealthfront.com.

Cash account offered by Wealthfront Brokerage LLC, member FINRA SIPC, and is not a bank.

APY on deposits as of September 26th, 2025 is representative, subject to change, and requires no minimum.

Funds are swept to program banks where they earn the variable APY.