Science vs The Supernatural: Does Science Kill the Magic?
Robin Ince and Brian Cox are joined on stage by actor and magician Andy Nyman, psychologist Richard Wiseman and neuroscientist Bruce Hood as they take on the paranormal. They'll be looking at some of the more popular claims of supernatural goings on, and asking whether a belief in ghosts, psychic abilities and other other-worldly phenomena, is just a bit of harmless fun, or whether there are more worrying implications in a belief in the paranormal.
Listen and follow along
Transcript
Hello, Heath Robins.
And he's Brian Cox, and this is Infinite Monkey Cage.
Today I'm going to test my psychic abilities with this audience here.
Now, did anyone in the audience know anyone who owned a hat?
Yes.
Excellent.
So there we are.
That's proved already that I have an uncanny ability to commune with the dead
or at least make loose statements that are easily applicable to many different people.
It's a gift.
We're actually going to be looking at science and the supernatural.
We aim to list every paranormal phenomenon that has occurred throughout all of human history that cannot be explained by science.
Good night.
But why do at least three out of four living people have a belief in the supernatural?
And that figure goes through the roof when you ask dead people.
Thank you, Andrew.
That was more effective than I imagined.
Much more.
To help us flesh out the answer beyond the negative, we have a panel panel of experts or the nearest available equivalent.
Our first guest has spent the last year terrifying audiences in his hit play ghost stories.
He's also fought zombies and lost in Charlie Brooker's Dead Set and devised tricks for Darren Brown.
He's an all-rounder.
Well, I say all-rounder, all-rounder on a sphere made of zombies, Russian roulette, and parapsychology.
In fact, he's terrible at typing and not particularly impressed with volleyball, but nevertheless, it's Andy Nyman.
Our next guest started as a young magician and remains obsessed by the nature of illusion and people's fascination with the paranormal.
As a psychologist and author, he has asked many important questions: like how can we find happiness?
Why does the human mind appear to trick itself?
And is that a duck?
It's Professor Richard Wiseman.
And our final guest is a neuroscientist at the University of Bristol and author of Super Sense: Why We Believe in the Unbelievable.
His next book will investigate if free will is an illusion.
Why anyone would choose to write about that?
I've got absolutely no idea.
It's Professor Bruce Hood, and that's our panel.
So, all of you, in some ways, have a reasonable knowledge there of superstition.
I want to start with you, Bruce.
What is it about the human that means so many people, in fact the majority of people, still seem to require a belief in the supernatural?
I think it's really part and parcel of our makeup.
I suppose there are two schools of thought, that they're either very gullible and believe stories which are passed on through culture.
Or my position is that there is actually an inclination in all of us to try and interpret events in the world.
And in doing so, sometimes we come up with explanations which resonate with beliefs in spirits and ghosts and all manner of things which scientifically are not viable.
Andy, in your play, Ghost Stories, you begin with a lecture, a kind of, I suppose, a history of the ghost story.
And you point out there that it's something that's evolved over the centuries.
It didn't begin as what we recognise as a ghost story.
No, well, the first historically recorded ghost stories we found were in 600 AD in Pope Gregory the Great's dialogues.
And what happened to those was that was the first time really the church saw something that they recognised as, oh, that's useful,
we can have that and make people feel that they should behave.
Otherwise, these, you know, the very notion of a spirit and an idea being written down was a powerful weapon to allow people to actually behave as they should, in inverted commas, behave.
And then after the Reformation, when the Catholic Church sort of lost its power, what happened was the idea of the notion of a ghost story became a secular idea,
where it was a way of being able to cope with tragedy and being able to frame terrible things that happened in your life.
So it meant then that those ghost stories that we tell each other could stop being something that was just tied to the church and could now become about real life.
And, you know, oh, I saw that soldier that had been killed in the
whatever it was.
So that was sort of the lineage, really, an early lineage of how the written ghost story came to evolve.
And Richard, is so the ghost story, is that the most common cultural experience of the paranormal today?
It's certainly one of the most common.
So, we did a survey recently, and one in four people experienced, or said they'd experienced, some ghost-like phenomena.
And of those experiences, the most common one is what
people refer to as the incubus, which is the little demonic figure that comes in the middle of the night and sits on your chest, pins you down, makes you sexually aroused, and scares you.
At least that's what he does to me.
I don't know about
I now realise that I could be over-generalizing my own experiences and no one else.
But for those of you listening on the radio, he did that purely with the look on his face.
But no, the look on your face did not suggest that you were the victim.
The look on your face suggested a man who occasionally dons a sheet and thinks, I'll go into that room.
A man who's using psychology, possibly for evil.
Yes, again.
Anyway, so the
incubus, which has been around a long time, basically
takes very many different forms.
And so if you're into alien abduction, you think it's an alien that's sitting on your chest.
And if you're into ghosts, then it's a spirit rather than a demonic force like the incubus.
So
what's interesting about the experience is you find it in pretty much every different culture throughout time.
And there's a very good psychological explanation for it.
I don't know what it is, but...
No, I do.
It's that when we dream, we become paralyzed, so we don't act out our dreams and injure ourselves.
And as we drift from dream state into waking state, some of the bizarre imagery comes across, and so does the notion of being paralyzed.
And you try and interpret that, you try and make sense of it.
And you think, oh my goodness, I'm paralyzed, must be somebody holding me down.
Now, oh my goodness, I see this evil spirit, that's what's happening there.
And what's really interesting is that that experience that can be terrifying for people if you buy into the idea of demonic forces or ghosts or aliens, once you explain the psychology, is actually not at all scary.
And so it can help people to know the psychological explanation for it.
So, some people who've had this, people can actually kind of embrace that, you know, that the cure for it and will take on reason in this, because obviously, you know, the battle of reason against superstition is a difficult one.
Absolutely, same with hearing voices.
I mean, if you hear voices, you think it's the voice of God or the dead, that's scary.
As soon as you know that our brain produces internal dialogue for all sorts of reasons, actually, it's not so scary.
So, in that sense, being rational can help you.
Saying that, there are genuine demonic forces out there,
which I imagine your next book will probably deal with.
My next book, Genuine Demonic Forces.
See you in your room later.
I'll be in the sheets.
But Bruce, so this, Richard, there has given an explanation for the, I suppose, the appearance of ghosts or the perception of ghosts
in the space between wake and sleep, sleep and wake.
So is that the general explanation for these, I suppose, misfirings of the cognitive system?
People who are inclined towards these notions tend to see structure and order in otherwise ambiguous patterns.
There have been experiments showing that they will see structure and patterns and what is effectively random noise.
Why are we predisposed to do that?
Well, I suppose it's really an evolutionary strategy is to assume the worst, which is possibly another agent or a predator in the bushes or somebody who's about to beat you over the head and steal your food or your mate.
And therefore you always tripwire to assume there's an agent out there to do you in.
So it's a better strategy to assume there's something there rather than ignore it.
And Andy, is that the basis of the success of ghost stories throughout the ages?
That we're pre-programmed to be scared because we also seem to enjoy it.
Oh, I think we absolutely love ghost stories.
I think, well, most people love the idea of being scared and love.
There's a very something absolutely life-affirming about going to a horror film or a play in the West End called Ghost Stories running till July the
16th, perhaps,
at Juki York's Theatre.
Bless you for that.
But yeah, there's absolutely, I'm sure, some sort of primitive drive within us that the choice to believe isn't always, sometimes it's so near the surface, it's amazing.
And a couple of years ago, there used to be a hilarious psychic hour on a London radio station.
And I was listening to it, and there was a woman on there who was a phone in, and she'd phoned into the psychic and said, This extraordinary thing happened.
I don't know that you're going to be able to explain this.
She said, I live at home with my cats, and not making it up, but I felt like saying,
so at home with my cats, and I have a piano.
And I'd gone to bed, and the piano was down.
And
I went to bed, and I suddenly heard four or five notes playing on the piano.
And I came back down and had a look, and the thing was down, and it was, you know, couldn't have been the cats.
So they had a sceptic on there as well.
And the sceptic said to her, look, well, one of the interesting things is that pianos, because it's a string instrument, you you know, fundamentally, and that they contract and expand in heat.
And sometimes you'll find in changes of temperature that you will, it sounds like notes are playing.
And she went,
and then the psychic said, The other thing is that fairies play notes.
And she went, Yes, yes, I think they play.
What's extraordinary is that the desire to believe it is so overwhelming that all logic flies out of the window to just grasp onto something magical.
But to get back to your very good point about whether we enjoy being scared, I actually don't think we do.
I think what's interesting about plays and about stories is we know they're not real.
It is that you can explore the notion of the ghost in a relatively safe environment.
But actually, when you do see a ghost, as it were, you are very scared, and that isn't quite such an enjoyable experience.
And to me, it's the same as going on a roller coaster.
That you're being thrown around, and it's all rather fun because you know it's safe.
As a good practical joke, if you go on roller coasters, take on like a rusty bolt and then just as it's just as it's moving off, just palm it and take it out and then say to your friend, Oh, is this supposed to come out?
Because
then they experience genuine fear.
But this is something that I find because I was, as I imagine, actually, most of this panel today, who were all somewhere in their 40s, brought up in a world of unexplained magazine, Arthur C.
Clarke's mysterious world.
We were surrounded by ideas of ESP, Bigfoot, Loch Ness, spontaneous combustion.
And yet, you know, none of us, as far as I know, here have clung on to those ideas, even though they would have been put into us when we were quite young.
But they're all gorgeous ideas, though, aren't they?
That's what makes them so seductive, is they're all, as you say all of those, you just feel, God, Bigfoot, don't you wish that was real?
The myth of it is so great and nessy.
And I think that's really part of it:
it's almost like great advertising.
You know, they're so perfect, those ideas, that they feel.
How could that not be real?
It's just brilliant.
And you almost want it to be.
But did you have a moment, Andy, where you just, you know, just the way the passion you talk about it now, where you kind of reached a certain age and you went, I don't think there is a Bigfoot.
And I don't think I'm going to take the train to Scotland because I don't think at the most there'll be a jumping otter.
I've never had that moment.
No, no, I've never had that moment.
I still sort of, whilst I am deeply sceptical and, you know, I have, I believe what I believe, but no, I mean, there's still that little tiny bit of me that wishes on the news tomorrow, they go, Big Foot's been found, look, here it is.
You'd think, oh, that's brilliant.
Watch Fox News.
It will greatly increase that chance, I think.
See, that then interests me.
Is there something, though, something in the wiring where, you know, there are lots of things that I would love to believe in, but I just go, no, it can't be.
And it's not just about reading.
It's not just about education, because there's people i know who have read widely and still cling to beliefs that i wouldn't be able to make that leap so is there something actually within us well there's a couple of things i mean first of all we tend to believe what we would like to be true francis bacon said that about you know rationality in general that we prefer to believe what
what we would like and that includes an afterlife but yeah i i happen to think that there is a real difference between people who are very much inclined to sort of the woo as we call it because they do tend to score very similarly on on measures of personality.
They're more creative, they're more of that kind of notion.
I'm not suggesting that it's black and white, but there does seem to be a profile which fits with this kind of way of thinking.
I know, Richard, actually, you have an experiment you could do on the audience to try and seek out people of that persuasion.
We could do that.
We could do a little test here of essentially imagination.
So, whatever.
Close your eyes, he'll put a sheet on.
I'll work through the group one by one.
So, yes, everyone can try this and try this at home as well.
What you need to do is hold out your hands straight in front of you, palm down.
So, a kind of Superman flying pose.
So, both hands out, stretched out in front of you, palms down, and level.
It's very important.
Excellent.
And now, don't consciously move your hands, but all you need to do is close your eyes and listen to my voice.
And just imagine a helium balloon, a very light balloon, attached to a string, and the end of that string attached to the fingers of your right hand.
So, your right hand being pulled up towards the ceiling, gently being tugged up by that helium balloon.
Then there's the heavy books, another set of heavy books attached to another string attached to the fingers of your left hand.
So your left hand being pulled down towards the floor, your right hand being pulled up towards the ceiling.
Just focus on my voice as you imagine that right hand being pulled up, left hand being pulled down.
Don't consciously move your hands, but keep them exactly as they are.
And now, if you open your eyes,
excellent.
That was amazing, which is because every single person in the audience did that very markedly, the difference in the hands, every single one of them.
And
now, you were saying there's a reason this experiment's never been done on television.
No, what you had there were around about 10% of the audience being highly suggestible, which means their hands, you know, the right hand is higher than the left one.
Most of you, being a rational, sceptical, scientific audience, your hands were level.
Some of you, I noticed, your hands were the other way round.
And that's just odd.
so what does that what does that mean well it's to do with suggestibility so the people whose hands were were markedly different the bad news is you're you're very suggestible if you go to a hypnosis show you'll probably end up on stage eating an onion naked
and the the the good news is you've got very good imaginations the people whose hands were level you're more down to earth you're less likely to see ghosts and have these weird paranormal experiences and and you have rubbish imaginations and you're not at all creative so
that simple test is a way of getting at imagination, and again, does split between the believers and the skeptics very well.
I'd just like to add, though, of course, you can increase this or enhance this effect by just putting people in stressful situations.
For example, if your plane starts to plummet at 35,000 feet, you'll start to become quite superstitious and ritualistic in your behavior.
And of course, just telling people they're in a situation where there's been a death in the house or that there's a loss of control, and they start to become more ritualistic, they become more superstitious.
So, it's an inclination that can be swayed by context very easily.
And is that in some sense when you're under pressure, you're stripping away parts of the rational mind in a sense?
I think when you're under pressure, you're under stress.
And one thing that we don't like is to be stressed.
And so we seek out control.
And when we believe we have control, either through our rituals or our processes of superstitious behaviors,
we feel we're controlling the situation.
That inoculates us against the worst excesses of stress.
I think that gets to the root of it, because I think that summarizes what's underneath the whole of the paranormal experiences, that we don't like uncertainty and we really don't like uncertainty in stressful situations and somebody comes along with a magical solution.
And of course there's the desire to believe.
If you've lost a loved one and somebody comes along and says actually you can still communicate with them, you know, or you're not well and somebody comes along and says actually you have a psychic healing, then it's very human to go, my goodness, that would be wonderful.
The problem here is there's no evidence to back up any of these things.
So you end up believing things that aren't true and actively damaging yourself and others in the process.
But I think that gets to the root of it: that we don't like uncertainty, particularly when we're stressed.
Well, you were just saying there about, but you mentioned psychic mediums there as well.
And is there actually a use in terms of do people say who've been to a psychic medium event, have they gained something?
Is there some good that they've gained out of that experience?
Well, of course, this is where the controversy really starts to rein in.
Yes, they may feel that they've gained something, but they've also lost a lot as well, which is usually financial.
And some people would argue that really, you know, we shouldn't be supporting these sub-belief systems, which potentially can have negative outcomes.
And I'm sure
Andy and Richard have both had this sort of experience.
To me, it isn't really about does the person who's receiving the reading gain anything.
If the person giving the reading is a sh what is known in the trade as a shot eye, someone who believes what they're giving, then to me, you know, whether they believe it or not, at least it's sort of given in good intention, as opposed to the coldness of
the taking money for this, the scam of that from a person known as an open eye who's a fraudulent one.
That to me, I just cannot get my head around how people can operate on that level.
And it is very difficult often to attach the appropriate weight to coincidence, isn't it, which lies at the heart of many of these belief systems.
I suppose with psychics,
the ones that really believe they're psychics, they will always say something that's close to something someone in an audience has experienced.
Well, that's it.
I mean, it's just that, you know, we're opening up a whole other area now, which is the fact that we all believe we're so different, whereas actually, the reality is we are all fundamentally exactly the same animals, be that living here or living in southern states of America, wherever it is.
So if you make a random open statement here, it's going to apply to X amount of people here as it will do there.
And as long as it's of a personal level, you feel
that's touched me.
How could you know that?
I think that the notion that we're more predictable than we think is crucial to the success of mediums.
And so when we all think, you know, if you take a very general statement, you know, you're an above-average driver in terms of safety, about 90% of people go, yes, I'm above-average driver.
You can go into hospital wards where people are in there because they're accidents that they themselves have caused whilst driving.
It's still 90% of them think they're above-average driver.
95% of us think we have an above-average sense of humor.
There's very few people that go, No, I just don't get it.
So,
we're more predictable, we think, and as a psychics come out with these relatively general things, like you have a lot of untapped creative potential, and we all go, yes, my goodness, how insightful, that's true.
And so, we don't realize that we are quite predictable and that people can use that then to allegedly tell things about us and they know nothing at all.
So, in terms of what people gain when they go to a medium or a psychic, I think they gain a greater belief in something that isn't true.
And that's pretty much it.
They might get some sense of comfort, but it's not going to be a long-term sense of comfort.
Counseling, mainstream counselling, is about giving you the tools to solve this problem and future problems, so you don't have to keep on coming back to the counsellor.
The psychic just gives you the solution and you become reliant on them.
That's very, very difficult.
There's also something we're beginning to, I'm so sorry, that we're just beginning to sort of see again and again, which is this notion of a sort of quick fix as well, isn't it?
There's an ease to these solutions as opposed to actually working for a solution.
Is there a sense in which
the belief in the paranormal appears to be growing?
I mean, it certainly feels that way.
And I know that you work Andy with Darren Brown a lot now.
I know that he's absolutely careful to say these are tricks.
However, many, many millions of people watch the shows.
So is there a sense in which the belief is growing in our scientific age that it's just not been suppressed at all?
There's an interesting thing.
I wrote with and sort of directed Derren stuff for 10 years, and the interesting thing about the Derren product, you know, as an idea, was that the NLP gurus said, this is the greatest, even though, you know, Derren stands there and says, this is magic, conjuring, science, hypnosis, whatever.
The NLP brigade said, this is the greatest form of NLP we've ever seen.
The psychics said, well, you can dismiss it, but you clearly have a gift.
And the magicians all said, this is fantastic magic.
I've never seen it.
So everybody kind of embraced the bits that they wanted to see.
But Richard and I worked together years ago.
We did a seance.
We did, and there we were staging a theatrical reconstruction of a seance, and we told people that's what it was, but still it was pretty terrifying for people because once you're sitting in the darkness holding hands with a stranger, it can be very scary.
And then you start to be sales.
But to answer this, it's
subtext to your work.
There is, there is.
I just remove the word sub.
At this point, yeah.
It's the.
No, mate, let's not go there.
So the
anyway, so to answer your question about belief in the paranormal,
it doesn't actually seem to be on the rise.
But
it's always pretty high.
It's around about a third of people claim to have experienced any of these weird things, ASP, telepathy, ghosts, and so on.
And that's stayed pretty stable.
You see it bumping up and down a little bit.
And that suggests to me at least there's something there which is keeping that thing stable.
And when you get, you know, all the sort of paranormal programming piling in and saying all this stuff is true, it goes up.
And when you get more scepticism on TV, it goes down a little bit.
But it's pretty much bumbling along there for the last sort of 50 years or so.
I suppose to bring this discussion to a close, there's a sense in which we've sat here and we've tried to understand this phenomenon that isn't there and we've had this clear view, which we would have because it's an infinite monkey cage, a bastion of rational thought.
Is there any harm in it?
I mean, because does it matter?
Does it matter if, as you said, a third of the population believe in the paranormal?
Many people have had paranormal experiences.
Are we supposed to be trying to cure them in a medical sense?
I think you just have to be on your guard.
It's the consequences of these beliefs is what we should be paying attention to.
People who have these beliefs can be kind of generally kind of kooky types of people.
And, you know, creativity,
a lot of the artists, a lot of musicians, a lot of very creative people have all these kind of ways of thinking about the world.
I don't want to dissuade them from those kind of beliefs, but if they start to impact on their health and the health of others and the well-being of others, then clearly they've got to be stopped.
I think it's enormously important because people go to psychics and you wouldn't turn up to buy a second-hand car knowing nothing about cars at all.
You'd know you'd get ripped off.
So you'd either learn or you take somebody with you.
Yet you go people go to a psychic, they're not informed consumers.
They say, Here's the problem.
They're there because they have a serious problem, financial or relationship or health or whatever.
And then the psychic does something which appears to be amazing, and then the person follows that advice.
And you're sitting opposite somebody who's got no counselling qualifications, because anyone can set them up as a psychic themselves up as a psychic, and still people are sitting there taking the advice.
So I think there really is a problem.
It's a huge industry in this country, and yet people have no idea what they're up against.
Yes, these plays about ghost stories and things make a fortune.
If only that were true.
By the way, I found the most interesting on this panel is having people who are all based in psychology, I really expected some subtle advertising for everything you were doing.
And finding out that the simplest form is just to mention your book will take a closing date.
It is.
What a relief to find out all those courses I've been on were a waste of time.
Yeah, I haven't mentioned mine.
Oh, that's what you say.
But if we replay this tape now,
if we take every second syllable, you will find quirk on the gee.
So there we are.
What's going to happen to everyone listening?
If you play this backwards, you'll hear a voice saying, Richard Wiseman, 799, Amazon.
ISBN 3210.
See, now you've got, because we were talking about this afternoon, you decided that ghosts definitely didn't exist, which is unusual for you, because normally you kind of, you know, earth aside of caution.
But you had a specific argument, if anyone says the ghost, from the perspective of physics, why it's ridiculous.
Not just generally from
why the laws of physics mean that ghosts are far less likely than we might have thought.
Well, I think they're perpetual motion machines.
I mean, they violate the laws of thermodynamics, as far as I can see.
I mean, we process information by eating food, and there's an energy source and a temperature difference, and that's the way that our brain works.
And once you turn that off, I don't understand how.
The reason I don't understand it, by the way, is because it's rubbish.
I don't know why I'm explaining it.
But they are explaining.
Nevertheless, your scientific mind couldn't go, which just seems absolutely ridiculous.
There'd There'd be a kind of see-through-y version of you that would drift around and go, and not only that, the laws of thermodynamics, which are acceptable for some zombies, but not those who've actually lost their stomach lining.
Yeah, yeah.
So we asked the audience what was their most ridiculous paranormal experience, and here are some of the answers.
This is from Martin.
Having a medium explain to me how his wireless modem was possessed and needed an exorcism.
My cat died, my dad could still hear it purring.
It turned out to be the boiler.
And was that the boiler that killed the cat?
My hamster made a crop circle in its bedding.
That was from G.
Host.
My daughter told us one night the house mooed.
There's one here, seeing Albert Albert Einstein sitting in my seat the night before my final physics exam.
There was a slight ooh of plausibility there that I didn't like from the audience.
Hopefully, now that is everything explained about ghosts, but we should still end on a warning.
Though we may have at times appeared frivolous in this show, please do not attempt to summon the dead or mess with the dark arts as they frequently stain.
And if any of the issues in this show have affected you, then.
Oh, don't be so silly.
Thank you to our panel, Andy Nyman, Richard Wiseman, and Bruce Hood.
That's the end of the series.
We'll be back in November.
Until then,
stay rational.
They wanted us to be a bit more rational.
Brian, do you really think we're going to come back in November?
I want to know.
I'll have a look at the tarot cards.
That doesn't look very very good.
Robin, it's dark in here with the lights out.
Of course, it's dark, Brian.
My plan has worked.
You believe in science, and now you are mine.